An Update on Media Multitasking

Credit: skynesher/iStock.com
Credit: skynesher/iStock.com

Can we talk about the perils of media multitasking enough? I don’t think so, and that belief was confirmed by a clearly organized and well-written review of the research on multitasking with various electronic devices (May & Elder, 2018). I try to be measured in my response to articles on teaching and learning, saving claims of greatness for only the best—and this is one of the best. Even better, it appears in an open-access source.

Teaching Professor Blog

Most (maybe it’s close to all) of us are well-aware that multitasking in and out of class compromises students’ efforts to learn. We don’t need to be persuaded, but students do, and we need facts to convince them. This article includes many that should. Here are some of its evidence-based conclusions.

Students are media multitasking in class and when they study. In a 2012 survey (Junco), 69 percent of students said they texted during class, and 88 percent reported using Facebook and email. Analysis of time logs completed in 3,372 self-directed computer sessions by 1,249 students revealed that 99 percent of the sessions involved some multitasking (Judd, 2014).

Media multitasking lowers GPA. Fifteen studies cited in the article report that result. The more that media are used, the larger the negative effect on GPA, and gender is a nonsignificant variable. It doesn’t matter whether multitasking occurs during class or while studying; the effect is the same: lower GPAs.

Media multitasking in class results in lower test scores. Use of phones has been studied here. For example, in a study where students listened to a videotaped lecture, those not multitasking with media had 62 percent more information in their notes and scored a full grade and a half higher on a posttest (Kuznekoff & Titsworth, 2013). Texting lowers quiz scores as well. In one study (Froese et al., 2010), texting students “lost roughly 30% of accuracy on a quiz when texting.” Students who use laptops are frequently distracted by them. Kraushaar and Novak (2010) report that students opened 65 windows per lecture, and 62 percent of them contained irrelevant content. Distractive laptop window use also lowers exam scores. Here, as well, 15 studies document that using media while listening to lectures compromises exam performance as well as note-taking and content comprehension.

Laptop multitasking hinders the learning of nearby peers. Students who could directly view the laptop of a multitasking peer scored 17 percent lower on a multiple-choice comprehension test than students unable to see a multitasking peer (Sana et al., 2013).

Media multitasking reduces efficiency while studying. In one study (Subrahmanyam et al., 2013) students read two passages (one hard, one easy) while either multitasking or not. Reading comprehension was not affected, but “multitaskers demonstrated markedly reduced efficiency.” Students who said they texted while studying reported spending more time studying than those who didn’t text (Bellur et al., 2015).

Students underestimate the negative impact of multitasking with media. Multiple studies report this finding. In one (Clayson & Haley, 2012), 68 percent of the students believed they could pay attention to a lecture and text at the same time. In another study students reported that multitasking increased their productivity (Lin et al., 2015).

In some of the work reviewed, students were aware of multitasking’s harmful effects. For example, when asked to predict performance on a quiz, those who texted expected lower quiz scores and were less confident about their predictions (Gingerich & Lineweaver, 2014). But despite recognizing that multitasking divides attention, students do it anyway.

So what can teachers do? Tell students to stop and enforce it with a strict policy? Maybe that will decrease the amount of media multitasking in the course, but forced compliance doesn’t teach self-regulation skills or stop the multitasking outside of class. How media multitasking affects learning is something students need to discover for themselves. But teachers can confront students with the kind of evidence contained in this review and then challenge them to find out for themselves. Many of the activities used in these studies could be replicated in a classroom.

Rather than reference all the studies cited in the column, I again recommend consulting the article. The citations are there, plus many more nuggets that you can share shared with students—in class, on the syllabus, on course materials, in the office, on the course website, in an email, or in any creative way that gets their attention.

Reference

May, K. E., & Elder, A. D. (2018). Efficient, helpful, or distracting? A literature review of media multitasking in relation to academic performance. International Journal of Educational Technology in Higher Education, 15. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41239-018-0096-z (open access)

Leave a Reply

Logged in as Julie Evener. Edit your profile. Log out? Required fields are marked *

This site uses Akismet to reduce spam. Learn how your comment data is processed.

Related Articles

Love ’em or hate ’em, student evaluations of teaching (SETs) are here to stay. Parts <a href="https://www.teachingprofessor.com/free-article/its-time-to-discuss-student-evaluations-bias-with-our-students-seriously/" target="_blank"...

Since January, I have led multiple faculty development sessions on generative AI for faculty at my university. Attitudes...
Does your class end with a bang or a whimper? Many of us spend a lot of time crafting...

Faculty have recently been bombarded with a dizzying array of apps, platforms, and other widgets that...

The rapid rise of livestream content development and consumption has been nothing short of remarkable. According to Ceci...

Feedback on performance has proven to be one of the most important influences on learning, but students consistently...

wpChatIcon